In a recent post, I gave a brief overview of the use of
computerized neurocognitive tests in the management of sports-related
concussions. I mentioned that while
these tests are standard practice, they are far from perfect. Now, I’ll expand on that statement.
As I discussed before, these tests are used to identify
subtle problems that can’t be detected using standard neuroimaging techniques
or medical exams. The thinking is that
if we can identify these impairments, we can decrease risk of subsequent (and
potentially more serious) injuries.
However, factors relating to the tests we use, the athletes who we
administer them to, and characteristics of the injuries themselves can severely
impair these tests’ abilities to correctly identify individuals who are still
suffering from the effects of concussions.
In terms of tests such as ImPACT, studies have generally
found low-to-moderate test-retest reliability, suggesting that athletes tend
not to perform consistently when they take the test multiple times (see Broglio,
Ferrara, Macciocchi, Baumgartner, & Elliott, 2011; Elbin, Schatz, & Covassin, 2011). This is a major problem, considering that the
tests are designed to be administered to the same athlete multiple times. One contributor to this issue is the presence
of practice effects, whereby athletes improve their performance due simply to
having taken the test before. Another
factor hampering reliability may be that the construct itself (i.e., impairment
due to sports concussion) can’t be reliably measured due to its often subtle
and transient nature.
The way in which the tests are used can also hurt their
validity. An advantage of these tests is
their ability to be administered to groups of athletes, saving organizations
time and money. In fact, most baseline
testing is done in groups. On the other
hand, post-injury testing is done on an individual basis. Given evidence that group testing may result
in poorer performance than individual testing (Moser, Schatz, Neidzwski, & Ott,
2011), comparisons of performance on baseline testing (group setting) to
post-injury testing (individual setting) may not be accurate.
Athletes are generally expected to play through injury. As a result, they may purposefully distort
results by engaging in “sandbagging.” Sandbagging is when someone purposefully
performs poorly on the baseline test, which resulting in a low score that is
easier to equal on post-injury testing (Peyton Manning has admitted
to doing this). The computerized
tests do have ways of identifying this; however, little research has been done
to determine if they work.
Finally, it remains to be seen if the baseline testing model
is effective in reducing the risk of either subsequent concussions or more
serious long-term effects. One study
found that a symptom-free waiting period did not reduce the risk of sustaining
another concussion (McCrea et al., 2009).
Although computerized neurocognitive tests are far from perfect, it is important to remember that they are just one tool in making return-to-play decisions after athletes suffer concussions. Physical exams, self-reported symptoms, and balance testing are also used. Furthermore, as objective measures go, these tests are the best we currently have. Consequently, their use will continue to be widespread until something better comes along.
References
Broglio, S. P., Ferrara, M. S., Macciocchi, S. N.,
Baumgartner, T. A., & Elliott, R. (2007). Test-retest reliability of
computerized concussion assessment programs. Journal of Athletic Training, 42, 509-514.
Elbin, R. J., Schatz, P., & Covassin, T. (2011).
One-year test-retest reliability of the online version of ImPACT in high school
athletes. The American Journal of Sports
Medicine, 39, 2319-2324.
McCrea, M., Guskiewicz, K., Randolph, C., Barr, W. E., Hammeke, T. A., Marshall, S. W., & Kelly, J. P. (2009). Effects of a symptom-free waiting period on clinical outcome and risk of reinjury after sport-related concussion. Neurosurgery, 65, 876-883.
Moser, R. S., Schatz, P., Neidzwski, K., & Ott, S. D.
(2011). Group versus individual administration affects baseline neurocognitive
test performance. American Journal of
Sports Medicine, 39, 2325-2330.
No comments:
Post a Comment